Blog post
Birth to three matters for schools: Attachment theory and readiness to learn
The role of attachment theory in education has frequently been questioned, as typified in the recent Twitter exchange on ‘edutwitter’ below.
Figure 1: Anonymised screenshot of a tweet and replies, Twitter, 2 September 2019
A growing body of research now indicates that attachment problems in early childhood affect the stress settings within the brain and the endocrine system, which can have a lasting impact not only upon human behaviour but also upon readiness to learn. This was explored in my article, ‘Attachment theory, cortisol and care for the under-threes in the twenty-first century: Constructing evidence-informed policy differences in the quality of early care’, recently published in the Early Years journal (Jarvis, 2020).
‘Attachment problems in early childhood affect the stress settings within the brain and the endocrine system, which can have a lasting impact not only upon human behavior but also upon readiness to learn.’
Attachment theory was created from a synthesis of psychoanalytic and evolutionary concepts. Bowlby (1952) proposed that babies have a natural, evolved instinct to form a strong emotional bond with their mothers, which if weak or missing would result in lifelong emotional problems. He linked this to natural selection, in that attachment-seeking protects the individual from danger. While Bowlby’s exclusive focus on the mother was robustly challenged, his internal working model (IWM) endures. The IWM is a mental ‘template’ upon which the infant constructs both a level of ‘lovability’ in the self-image and a model of predicted responses from others. If early relationships are affectionate, the IWM expectation becomes, ‘Other people are usually kind and I am lovable;’ but if relationships are troubled, the setting created is, ‘Other people are unpredictable, not always kind and I may not be lovable’ – and in extreme cases, ‘Other people are unkind and I am not lovable’.
Attachment theory was recently enhanced by a range of studies that discovered abnormally raised levels of the stress hormone cortisol in young children in non-familial care. The ongoing picture is complex, indicating that stress levels are differentially impacted by transactions between care quality in the family versus care quality in non-familial settings, the age of the child and the balance of time spent between domestic and professional environments.
The effects of excess cortisol in the human body are not constrained to social functioning, however: it ‘leaks’ into cognitive functions, and in so doing has far-reaching effects upon children’s readiness to learn. Raymond and colleagues (Raymond, Marin, Majeur, & Lupien, 2018, p. 152) explain:
‘The main brain structures that are affected by the chronic secretion of stress hormones during childhood (hippocampus, prefrontal cortex and amygdala) are differentially involved in various cognitive functions (memory, emotion regulation, encoding of emotional memories, etc.).’
Wagner et al. (2016) further linked higher cortisol levels to poorer ‘executive functioning’ across three cognitive domains: inhibitory self-control, flexibility and emergent metacognition. The impact that this has upon human beings in a working or learning environment are summarised by Scorza and colleagues (Scorza, Arayad, Wuermlib, & Betancourt, 2016, 34):
‘Executive function is an umbrella term that refers to the following mental processes: working memory or the capacity to hold and manipulate information over short periods of time, inhibitory control or the ability to master and filter thoughts and impulses and to pause and think before acting, and cognitive or mental flexibility such as the ability to shift attention between tasks.’
Within the school environment, the following features are commonly observed in children with a history of troubled family relationships (Jarvis, 2020):
- fight-or-flight response always on stand-by
- quick to anger, to sadness and ‘learned helplessness’
- short attention span
- problems concentrating at school
- views the world as a dangerous place
- mistrustful of adults and other children
- feeling of inadequacy/lacks confidence
- lack of self-belief/lack of self-motivation
- may alternate between overdependence upon and rejection of others.
These findings are surely of general interest to schools, raising questions relating to the necessity of pastoral support, alongside the inadvisability of harsh discipline regimes.
References
Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/40724
Jarvis, P. (2020). Attachment theory, cortisol and care for the under-threes in the twenty-first century: Constructing evidence-informed policy. Early Years – An International Journal of Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2020.1764507
Raymond, C., Marin, M-F., Majeur, D., & Lupien, S. (2018). Early child adversity and psychopathology in adulthood: HPA axis and cognitive dysregulations as potential mechanisms. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 85, 152–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.07.015
Scorza, P., Arayad, R., Wuermlib, A., & Betancourt T. (2016). Towards clarity in research on ‘non-cognitive’ skills: Linking executive functions, self-regulation, and economic development to advance life outcomes for children, adolescents and youth globally. Human Development, 58(6), 313–317. https://doi.org/10.1159/000443711
Wagner, S., Cepeda, I., Krieger, D., Maggi, S., D’Angiulli, A., Weinberg, J., & Grunau, R. (2016). Higher cortisol is associated with poorer executive functioning in preschool children: The role of parenting stress, parent coping and quality of daycare. Child Neuropsychology, 22(7), 853–869. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1080232